教师资格证

导航

2016上半年教师资格考试《高中英语学科知识》真题与答案

来源 :考试网 2017-08-22

  请阅读 Passage l,完成第 21~25小题。

  Passage l .

  When the Viaduct de Millau opened in the south of France in 2004, this tallest bridge in the world won worldwide accolades. German newspapers described how it “floated above the clouds” with “elegance and lightness” and “breathtaking” beauty. In France, papers praised the “immense” “concrete giant.” Was it mere coincidence that the Germans saw beauty where the French saw heft and power? Lera Borodisky thinks not.

  In a series of clever experiments guided by pointed questions, Boroditsky is amassing evidence that, yes, language shapes thought. The effect is powerful enough, she says, that “the private mental lives of speakers of different languages may differ dramatically,” not only when they are thinking in order to speak, “but in all manner of cognitive tasks,” including basic sensory perception. “Even a small fluke of grammar”—the gender of nouns—“can have an effect on how people think about things in the world,” she says.

  As in that bridge, in German, the noun for bridge, Brucke, is feminine. In French, pont is masculine. German speakers saw prototypically female features; French speakers, masculine ones. Similarly, Germans describe keys (Schlussel) with words such as hard, heavy, jagged, and metal, while to Spaniards keys (llaves) are golden, intricate, little, and lovely. Guess which language construes key as masculine and which as feminine? Grammatical gender also shapes how we construe abstractions. In 85 percent of artistic depictions of death and victory, for instance, the idea is represented by a man if the noun is masculine and a woman if it is feminine, says Boroditsky. Germans tend to paint death as male, and Russians tend to paint it as female.

  Language even shapes what we see. People have a better memory for colors if different shades have distinct names—not English’s light blue and dark blue, for instance, but Russian’s goluboy and sinly. Skeptics of the language-shapes-thought claim have argued that that’s a trivial finding, showing only that people remember what they saw in both a visual form and a verbal one, but not proving that they actually see the hues differently. In an ingenious experiment, however, Boroditsky and colleagues showed volunteers three color swatches and asked them which of the bottom two was the same as the top one. Native Russian speakers were faster than English speakers when the colors had distinct names, suggesting that having a name for something allows you to perceive it more sharply. Similarly, Korean uses one word for “in” when one object is in another snugly, and a different one when an object is in something loosely. Sure enough, Korean adults are better than English speakers at distinguishing tight fit from loose fit.

  Science has only scratched the surface of how language affects thought. In Russian, verb forms indicate whether the action was completed or not—as in “she ate [and finished] the pizza.” In Turkish, verbs indicate whether the action was observed or merely rumored. Boroditsky would love to run an experiment testing whether native Russian speakers are better than others at noticing if an action is completed, and if Turks have a heightened sensitivity to fact versus hearsay. Similarly, while English says “she broke the bowl” even if it smashed accidentally, Spanish and Japanese describe the same event more like “the bowl broke itself.” “When we show people video of the same event,” says Boroditsky, “English speakers remember who was to blame even in an accident, but Spanish and Japanese speakers remember it less well than they do intentional actions. It raises questions about whether language affects even something as basic as how we construct our ideas of causality.”

  21. Which of the following is closest in meaning to the underlined word “accolades” in PARAGRAPH ONE?

  A. Praises.

  B. Awards.

  C. Support.

  D. Gratitude.

  22. What can be inferred from PARAGRAPH TWO?

  A. Language does not shape thoughts in any significant way.

  B. The relationship between language and thought is an age-old issue.

  C. The language we speak determines how we think and see the world.

  D. Whether language shapes thought needs to be empirically supported.

  23. What is the role of the underlined part “As in that bridge” in PARAGRAPH THREE?

  A. Reflecting on topics that appeal to the author and readers.

  B. Introducing new evidence to what has been confirmed before.

  C. Identifying the kinds of questions supported by the experiments.

  D. Claiming that speakers of different languages differ dramatically.

  24. Which of the following has nothing to do with the relationship between language and thought?

  A. People remember what they saw both visually and verbally.

  B. Language helps to shape what and how we perceive the world.

  C. Grammar has an effect on how people think about things around us.

  D. Science has only scratched the surface of how language affects thought.

  25. Which of the following best represents the author’s argument in the passage?

  A. The gender of nouns affects how people think about things in the world. .

  B. Germans and Frenchmen think differently about the Viaduct de Millau.

  C. Language shapes our thoughts and affects our perception of the world.

  D. There are different means of proving how language shapes our thoughts.

分享到

您可能感兴趣的文章